Authors: John W. Pilley
Chaser's memory for them was better than that. Following her retrieval of the objects, the five teams of students evaluated her accuracy. She had correctly retrieved forty-six of the fifty objects, or 92 percent. The entire Psychology Kingdom cheered Chaser's success.
Dave Pittman opened up the meeting for questions, and John Lefebvre, newly appointed chairman of the Psychology Department, asked if Chaser would retrieve objects for people other than me. I invited John to come down onto the stage and find out for himself.
John is not bashful, and in a minute he was on stage. Having sat in on several of John's classes, I knew he would test Chaser's limits. I threw eight of Chaser's toys on the floor, and asked John to have at it. He picked the smallest stuffed animal, a dog named Tiny. And then, out of Chaser's sight, he threw Tiny several steps up in the amphitheater. He turned to Chaser and said, “Chaser, find Tiny.”
Chaser quickly nosed through the objects on the floor. No Tiny. Although I thought the test was a little unfair because Chaser was in a strange environment and did not know John, I said nothing.
John repeated, “Chaser, find Tiny,” and Chaser began to explore the room. Several times she reexamined the objects on the floor. John wisely continued to repeat, “Chaser, find Tiny.” At least two minutes passed while Chaser looked all over the stage. Finally she approached the steps that led up through the rows of seats in the amphitheater, saw Tiny, and picked him up in her mouth.
Realizing that I was holding my breath, I heaved a huge sigh of relief. Again John showed his knowledge of dogs by enthusiastically praising Chaser. And the Psychology Kingdom of students clapped and roared their praise as if they were at a Wofford football game.
Dave Pittman was looking at the clock, but before he could bring the meeting to a close, a student asked for one more demonstration of Chaser's word learning. I said, “Chaser has learned the meaning of âclean up,' so I'm going to ask her to do that now. You will see, however, that she is like a two-year-old child and will need reminding to finish the task.” I turned to Chaser and said, “Chaser, clean up.” She quickly put two toys in the plastic tub but then dropped one at my feet. “No, Chaser,” I said. “It's time to clean up.” With that reminder, and a few more before we were done, Chaser put all eight toys in the tub. As the students clapped, a girl shouted, “Can Chaser clean my room?”
I said, “That depends on how well you motivate her. At home, there may be twenty toys on the floor. But if I tell Chaser, âClean up and we will play Frisbee,' she will put the toys in the tub in two minutes and race to the front door. Do you have a Frisbee?”
“Not yet,” she replied.
With laughter all around at that, Dave closed the meeting. If you're curious about the video, you can watch it at
www.youtube.com /pilleyjw
.
The demonstration gave me a big energy boost for working on the paper. In March, Alliston invited me to go with him to the Comparative Cognition Society's annual conference in Melbourne, Florida, where we would have an opportunity to report on Chaser's learning. The audience would be a tough-minded one, but receptive to the idea that a dog could reason. An increasing number of animal scientists around the world were moving away from Descartes's animals-are-just-meat-machines paradigm, and the conference attendees reflected that. I was too nervous to make the presentation, however, and I asked Alliston to do it. I was happy to be sitting in the third row listening to Chaser's story.
After Alliston's presentation several of the conference attendees asked tough questions, but in a friendly spirit that put me at ease. That evening at dinner I found myself sitting next to Clive Wynne. Alliston told me later that Clive asked to sit beside me because he was so intrigued by Chaser's learning. A transplanted Englishman, Clive was a full professor in the Psychology Department at the University of Florida's flagship campus in Gainesville, where he had his own Canine Cognition and Behavior Lab. He was also research director at Wolf Park, a research and public education facility in Battle Ground, Indiana, and editor in chief of the British journal
Behavioural Processes
.
Clive peppered me with questions about Chaser. He was fascinated to hear about the double-blind trial before Wofford's Psychology Club. And he was especially intrigued when I noted that Chaser performed language tasks for other trainers and questioners besides me.
“Suppose I were to come up to Spartanburg with a couple of students,” he said. “Would Chaser be able to complete language tasks as you and Alliston describe if the students and I were the ones asking her to do them?”
“Sure,” I said. “Come on ahead anytime.”
A few weeks later, Clive drove up to Spartanburg with two of his postdoctoral students and research collaborators, Monique Udell and Nicole Dorey. Sally, Chaser, and I welcomed them, and we spent a little time getting to know one another. Chaser's way of doing that, of course, was to engage Clive, Monique, and Nicole in playing with her. Each of them was happy to oblige. And then I showed them the Rubbermaid containers with Chaser's toys so that they could conduct their own trials.
Clive, Monique, and Nicole randomly picked twenty objects, which we took to Wofford. In the same auditorium where Chaser had demonstrated her learning for the Psychology Kingdom, Clive, Monique, and Nicole arranged the toys in random order behind a large screen. Monique and Nicole took turns asking Chaser to retrieve the objects by name, while Clive evaluated the accuracy of each selection.
When Chaser quickly brought the first object correctly, Clive, Monique, and Nicole all exchanged a look that said, “Ummm, this could be interesting.” With each retrieval, I could see their excitement growing.
Chaser brought the last of the objects out from behind the screen. She had not missed a single one. Clive turned to me and said, “If there's a trick to this, it's almost more impressive.”
Sally and I were both bursting with pride. To see a leading canine cognition researcher and two of his best postdocs bowled over after testing Chaser's learning for themselvesâwell, I was on cloud nine.
Back at our house we all sat down together on the porch, and Sally and I answered our visitors' many questions about Chaser's training. For our part, we were fascinated to hear about their ongoing research projects. Their main focus was on how wolves and domestic dogs compare in their sensitivity to people's gazes and gestures. Discovering the differences and similarities between wolves and dogs in this regard would shed light on the evolution of dogs' social intelligence. Meanwhile Chaser livened things up by luring each of them into her favorite games. And then Sally and I got a shock.
Just before Clive, Monique, and Nicole left to drive back to Gainesville, Clive asked, “Would you consider letting us take Chaser to our lab to work with her there and then write up our own study?”
Seeing the negative reaction on Sally's face and mine, Clive said, “I don't mean now, of course. But would you consider it for sometime in the future?”
In order to be polite we said we would, and we thanked him for that extraordinary vote of confidence in Chaser's learning. Over the next couple of weeks I had several talks with Clive about the possibility of Monique and Nicole's running a study with Chaser under his direction. The idea was tempting, because it would almost certainly assure that Chaser's learning would be shared with the scientific community at large through a peer-reviewed paper in a good journal. But there was really no way we could contemplate sending Chaser out of the family for weeks or months at a time.
Clive, Monique, and Nicole were all dog lovers. Sally and I had no doubt that they would take the best possible care of Chaser. But I finally explained to Clive that, as Sally and I always said, “Chaser is a member of our family.” Clive understood. He had already invited Alliston and me to submit our paper for possible publication in
Behavioural Processes
. He now took the further step of telling Julia Cort, a producer at PBS's
Nova scienceNow
, about Chaser.
In late September, Clive provisionally accepted Alliston's and my paper. The peer reviewers for
Behavioural Processes
wanted to see a number of revisions and elaborations of our procedures, but they were all things I knew we could handle.
In October, Julia Cort brought a teamâher assistant, a cameraman, and a soundmanâto Spartanburg to shoot a segment with Chaser for a “How Smart Are Animals?” feature on
Nova scienceNow
. They arrived the evening before the planned shoot. Tall and slender with shoulder-length dark brown hair, Julia was as congenial in person as she had been on the phone and in her e-mails, and her team were all as nice as they could be.
Neil deGrasse Tyson, the host of
Nova scienceNow
, who would be in the planned segment with Chaser, arrived the next morning. Although Chaser's ability to connect with all sorts of people never ceased to amaze me, I had been a little concerned about how she would react to Julia, Neil, and the crew. If the meeting was in any way bumpy, there would not be much time to put everyone at ease. I needn't have worried, however. Chaser loved them all, especially Neil, who delighted her with his infectious enthusiasm and sense of fun.
Neil and Julia were as blown away by Chaser's language learning as Clive and his students had been a few months earlier. They told us that the feature wouldn't be ready to show on
Nova scienceNow
for at least a year, because of the other segments they were planning. They also warned us that they couldn't promise what would actually wind up in the finished program. But there was no mistaking their excitement over the footage they'd recorded with Chaser.
Their response energized me. Now I couldn't wait to finish revising the paper and get final approval of it by
Behavioural Processes
. The snag was that Alliston's commitments to SQAB really ramped up over the 2009â10 academic year. But finally in the summer of 2010 we were able to get together on the final changes to the paper. Alliston outdid himself in crunching the data on Chaser's learning and presenting it in dramatic figures and tables. Allston's contributions really drove home the stringency of my criteria for whether Chaser had learned a word, and the magnitude of the testing I had done.
With regard to Chaser's proper noun learning, for example, we now had 8,000 1-of-8 tests, 1,000 8-of-8 tests, 838 20-of-20 tests, and 145 formal blind 20-of-20 tests to report, as well as the double-blind demonstration at Wofford. Statistics for each kind of test showed that the p-value, or probability-of-chance value, was always equal to or less than .004âwell below the point where Chaser's success could be attributed to chance. And the statistical evidence and blind and double-blind trials we presented on Chaser's other language learningâher combinatorial understanding of separate meanings for nouns and verbs, her grasp of common noun categories, and her ability to learn by exclusionâwere equally significant.
Clive sent us the peer reviewers' final comments, with suggestions for a few minor additional revisions, in early September. He also suggested a bold title: “Border Collie Comprehends Object Names as Verbal Referents.” It was not a title to excite the average person. But it was sure to grab the attention of any scientist with an interest in language learning by animals or children.
In November,
Behavioural Processes
formally accepted the paper, with e-publication to come the next month and print publication in February. And
Nova scienceNow
's “How Smart Are Animals” program, with Chaser as the center of a “How Smart Are Dogs” segment, was scheduled to broadcast in February.
The stars seemed to be aligning favorably, giving us a chance to reveal to the world that dogs are smarter than we often think.
13
C
HASER BECAME WORLD-FAMOUS
before we knew itâliterally.
It was early evening on Christmas Eve, 2010, and Sally and I were on the phone with Debbie in Brooklyn. Ten-year-old Aidan had just said good night after telling Sally and me what he was hoping Santa Claus would bring him for Christmas. Debbie said it was getting hard to hide presents from Aidan, but we were all happy that Christmas was still a magical time for him.
Debbie asked us what was new, and I remembered I had not yet told her that
Behavioural Processes
put Alliston's and my paper online on December 8. It had slipped my mind, and at any rate, the print publication wouldn't happen until February.
“Dad-d-d-d-d,” Deb said in half humorous, half serious exasperation. “Why didn't you mention that before? In today's world the online publication is probably just as important.”
I told her, “I don't know, honey. This is science, and the print edition will probably carry most of the weight.”
Deb sighed and asked, “Can you just give me the exact title of the paper? I'll Google it.” I did that, and we said good night.
Five minutes later the phone rang. It was Deb, practically breathless. “You won't believe what's happening!” she exclaimed. “When Jay and I Googled the name of the paper, all these links came up referring to Chaser as âthe world's smartest dog' and âthe dog with a thousand-word vocabulary.' Jay is scrolling through dozens and dozens of hits and he hasn't gotten anywhere near the end yet.”
Deb asked Jay to pick up the other phone to tell us what he was seeing. He came on the line and said, “Chaser's gone viral, John.”
Though I used the computer every day for e-mail, I wasn't Internet savvy and had to ask what that meant. Jay explained that news about Chaser was spreading on the Internet like a flu virus in a crowded room.
“Wow,” I whispered.
Deb said, “Oh my god, Dad, this is crazy!” There was silence for a few seconds, and then she repeated, “Oh my god. Tell Mom to pick up the other phone, or put on your speakerphone.”