How We Learn (18 page)

Read How We Learn Online

Authors: Benedict Carey

BOOK: How We Learn
10.76Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Most tried a few things, like pinning the candles to the door with the tacks, or fastening them with tape, before stalling out. But Duncker found that the success rate shot way up if he made one small adjustment: taking the tacks, matches, and other items
out
of the boxes. When the boxes were sitting on the table, empty, subjects saw that they could fasten those to the door with tacks, creating mini-platforms on which to mount the candles. Duncker hadn’t changed the instructions or the available materials one bit. Yet by emptying the boxes, he’d altered their mental representation. They were no longer merely
containers
, incidental to the problem at hand; they were seen as available for use. In Duncker’s terminology, when the boxes were full, they were “functionally fixed.” It was as if people didn’t see them at all.

This idea of fixedness infects our perceptions of many problems we encounter. We spend five minutes rifling through drawers searching for a pair of scissors to open a package when the keys in our pocket could do the job just as well. Mystery novelists are virtuosos at creating fixed ideas about characters, subtly prompting us to rule out the real killer until that last act (Agatha Christie’s
The Murder of Roger Ackroyd
is a particularly devious specimen of this). Fixedness is what makes the SEQUENC_ puzzle a puzzle at all: We make an automatic assumption—that the “_” symbol represents an empty space,
a
platform
for a letter—and it’s hard to shake that assumption precisely because we’re not even aware that we’ve made it.

Duncker ran comparison trials with all sorts of puzzles similar to the candle problem and concluded, “Under our experimental conditions, the object which is not fixed is almost twice as easily found as the object which is fixed.” The same principle applies, to some extent, in Maier’s pendulum experiment. Yes, the people trying to solve that problem first had to think of swinging the rope. Then, however, they had to devise a way to swing the rope far enough, by attaching the pliers. The pliers are pliers, a tool for squeezing things—until they become a weight for the pendulum. Until they become unfixed.

Between them, Maier and Duncker had discovered two mental operations that aid incubation, picking up clues from the environment, and breaking fixed assumptions, whether about the use of pliers, or the gender of a doctor. Here’s the rub: They had demonstrated those properties by helping their stumped subjects along with hints. Most of us don’t have a psychologist on call, ready to provide deskside incubation assistance whenever we’re stuck. We’ve got to make it happen on our own. The question is, how?

• • •

You’re shipwrecked. You swim and swim until finally you wash up on a desert island, a spit of sand no more than a mile around. As you stagger to your feet and scan the coastline, you realize: You’ve read about this place. It’s the Isle of Pukool, famous for its strange caste system. Members of the highest caste never tell the truth; members of the lowest always do; and those in the middle are sometimes honest and sometimes not. Outwardly, the castes are indistinguishable. Your only chance of survival is to reach the hundred-foot Tower of Insight, a holy site of refuge where you can see for miles and send out a distress signal. You follow a winding footpath and arrive at the one intersection on the island, where three Pukoolians are lounging in
the heat. You have two questions to ask (Pukool custom, you know) to find your way to that tower.

What do you ask?

I like this puzzle for several reasons. It captures the spirit of insight in a visceral way, for one. At first glance, it seems hairy—it echoes a famous problem in math logic, involving two guards and a man-eating lion
*2
—yet absolutely no math expertise is required. If anything, math expertise is likely to get in the way. A five-year-old can solve it. Better still, we can use it as a way to think about the most recent research on incubation and problem solving, which has branched out like a climbing vine since its duct-tape-and-thumbtack days.

To review, Wallas’s definition of incubation is a break that begins at the moment we hit an impasse and stop working on a problem directly, and ends with a breakthrough, the aha! insight. Maier and Duncker shone a light on what occurs mentally during incubation, what nudges people toward solutions. The question that then began to hang over the field in the last half of the twentieth century was
how
. Under what circumstances is incubation most likely to produce that aha! moment in real life? Wallas, Maier, and Duncker had incorporated breaks into their theories, but none specified how long of a break was ideal, or which
kind
of break was best. Should we hike in the woods, like Helmholtz? Go jogging for forty-five minutes? Stare into space? Some people prefer a nap, others a videogame. And there are students—I wish I were one of them—who will break from the knotty calculation they’re stuck on and turn to their history reading, a different species of break altogether. The religious reformer
Martin Luther is said to have had some of his deepest insights on the toilet, as did the prolific French essayist Michel de Montaigne. Should we be parking ourselves there when trying to incubate?

To try to answer these kinds of questions, psychologists have used old-fashioned trial and error. In more than one hundred experiments over the past fifty years, they have tested scores of combinations of puzzles, incubation durations, and types of study breaks. For instance, are people able to solve more anagrams when they take a five-minute break to play a videogame, or when they take a twenty-minute break to read? Daydreaming for a few minutes might be better than both, one study found; so might a Ping-Pong match. The most productive type of break might change entirely with other kinds of puzzles—riddles, rebus diagrams, spatial problems—and then change again when hints are given. This shifting, multidimensional experience is what scientists are trying to characterize in labs. One well-known experiment will illustrate how they do so.

This experiment, conducted by two psychologists at Texas A&M University named Steven Smith (whom we’ve met before) and Steven Blankenship, used a simple word puzzle called a
Remote Associates Test, or RAT. The subjects were given three words—“trip,” “house,” and “goal,” for example—and the challenge was to find a fourth that completed a compound word with each. (
Field
was the solution to this one: “field trip,” “field house,” and “field goal.”) Smith and Blankenship chose these puzzles in part because they could easily manipulate the level of difficulty by providing good hints, like “sports” for the example above (two of them are sports-related, and all you need is to find one and try it for the others) or bad hints, in the form of wrong answers, like “road,” which works with “trip” and “house” but not “goal.” The first kind of hint is akin to Maier’s swinging rope. The second is like Duncker’s filled boxes, creating a level of fixedness that is hard to overcome.

This experiment used the second kind, the bad clue. Smith and Blankenship wanted to know whether a short incubation break affects
people differently when they’re given bad hints—when they’re “fixed,” if you’ll excuse the expression—versus when they’re not. They recruited thirty-nine students and gave them twenty RAT puzzles each. The students were split into two groups. Half were given puzzles that had misleading words in italics next to the main clues (DARK
light
… SHOT
gun
… SUN
moon
), and the other half worked on the same puzzles, but without words next to the clues (DARK … SHOT … SUN). Both groups had ten minutes to solve as many puzzles as they could, and neither group did very well. Those who worked on the fixed ones solved two, on average, compared to five for the unfixed group.

The psychologists then gave their participants another ten minutes to work on the puzzles they hadn’t solved the first time through. This time around, each group was subdivided: half took the retest immediately, and the other half got a five-minute break, during which they read a science fiction story. So: Two groups, one fixed and one not. Two conditions within each group, incubation and no incubation.

The result? The incubation break worked—but only for those who got the bad clues. They cracked about twice as many of their unsolved puzzles as the unfixed group who got a break.

The authors attributed the finding to what they called “selective forgetting.” A fixating (misleading) word temporarily blocks other possible answers, they argued, but “as more time elapses, after the initial failed attempts, the retrieval
block may wear off.” It’s as if the students’ brains were temporarily frozen by the bad hints and the five-minute break allowed for some thawing out. This occurs all the time in normal daily life, most obviously when we get unclear directions—“the pharmacy is right at the end of Fowler Road, you can’t miss it”—and we arrive at the given spot, backtracking, circling, rechecking the street names: no pharmacy. We’re sure we’re missing it somehow. Finally, we sit down on a bench, stare at the birds for a few minutes, and it hits us:
Oh, wait: maybe he meant the
other
end of
Fowler Road
. Or, the pharmacy moved. Or he has no idea what he’s talking about. The initial assumption—the pharmacy must be around here, somewhere—no longer has a stranglehold on our mind. Other options have floated in. Romantic entanglements are another classic example: We become infatuated, we think we’re in love, but time loosens the grip of the fixation. We come to see exasperating flaws. Maybe she’s not the one, after all. What was I thinking?

In previous chapters, we’ve seen how forgetting can aid learning actively, as a filter, and passively, allowing subsequent study to ramp up memory. Here it is again, helping in another way, with creative problem solving.

As Smith and Blankenship were quick to note, selective forgetting is only one possible explanation for incubation,
in these specific circumstances
(RATs, fixed words, five-minute reading break). And theirs was just one experiment. Others have produced slightly different results: Longer breaks are better than shorter ones; playing a videogame is as good as reading; writing may help incubation for certain kinds of problems, such as spatial ones like the Pencil Problem. In each case—in each specific study—scientists have floated various theories about what’s happening in the buildup to that aha! moment. Maybe it’s selective forgetting. Maybe it’s a reimagining of the problem. Maybe it’s simple free-associating, the mind having had time to wander in search of ideas. No one knows for sure which process is the most crucial one, and it’s likely that no one ever will. Our best guess? They all kick in at some level.

What does that mean for us, then? How do we develop a study strategy, if scores of experiments are saying various, often contradictory, things?

To try to make sense of the cacophony, let’s return to the Isle of Pukool. How to find our Tower of Insight? The three Pukoolians are pointing in different directions, after all. It’s hard to know who’s being honest and who’s not.

What to do?

Easy.
Look up
. The tower is one hundred feet tall, and the island is flat, and the size of a city park. No complex math logic required: The tower is visible for miles. Try this on a group of friends when they’re in the mood. You’ll notice that some people see the answer right away, and others never come close. I didn’t come close. I spent hours concocting absurd, overly complex questions like, “Which way would those two fellow islanders say that you would say …?” I wrote out the various possible answers on paper, using a math notation I’d forgotten I knew. When I finally heard the solution, it seemed somehow unfair, a cheap trick. On the contrary. Taking a step back and
looking around
—seeing if we’ve used all the available information; attempting to shake our initial assumptions and start from scratch; doing a mental inventory—is a fitting metaphor for what we have to do to make sense of the recent work on incubation. Looking at each study individually is like engaging the Pukoolians one-on-one, or staring so closely at a stereogram that the third dimension never emerges. You can’t see the forest for the trees.

Thankfully, scientists have a method of stepping back to see the bigger picture, one they use when trying to make sense of a large number of varied results. The idea is to “pool” all the findings, positive and negative, and determine what the bulk of the evidence is saying. It’s called meta-analysis, and it sometimes tells a clearer story than any single study, no matter how well done. In 2009, a pair of psychologists at Lancaster University in the United Kingdom did precisely this for insight-related research, ransacking the available literature—even hunting down unpublished manuscripts—and producing a high-quality,
conservative meta-analysis. Ut Na Sio and Thomas C. Ormerod included thirty-seven of the most rigorous studies and concluded that the incubation effect is real, all right, but that it does not work the same in all circumstances.

Sio and Ormerod divided incubation breaks into three categories.
One was relaxing, like lying on the couch listening to music. Another was mildly active, like surfing the Internet. The third was highly engaging, like writing a short essay or digging into other homework. For math or spatial problems, like the Pencil Problem, people benefit from any of these three; it doesn’t seem to matter which you choose. For linguistic problems like RAT puzzles or anagrams, on the other hand, breaks consisting of mild activity—videogames, solitaire, TV—seem to work best.

Sio and Ormerod found that longer incubation periods were better than short ones, although “long” in this world means about twenty minutes and “short” closer to five minutes—a narrow range determined by nothing more than the arbitrary choices of researchers. They also emphasized that people don’t benefit from an incubation break
unless they have reached an impasse
. Their definition of “impasse” is not precise, but most of us know the difference between a speed bump and a brick wall. Here’s what matters: Knock off and play a videogame too soon and you get nothing.

Other books

Dark Journey by Stuart, Anne
Wisdom Seeds by Patrice Johnson
2 Death Makes the Cut by Janice Hamrick
Look to the Lady by Margery Allingham
This Is Paradise by Kristiana Kahakauwila
Salem Moon by Scarlet Black
Hogg by Samuel Delany