The Dolphin in the Mirror (22 page)

BOOK: The Dolphin in the Mirror
9.74Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Underwater windows in aquariums usually have some degree of reflectivity, like poor mirrors. But the pools at Vallejo had no such windows, and so for the first seven years of their lives, Delphi and Pan had had no experience with their own reflections. We were dealing with completely mirror-naive test subjects. Our immediate task was to see how they would behave when they first saw their reflections.

Dolphins are curious creatures, always ready to investigate novel objects in their environment. So it wasn't surprising that when we lifted the tarp from the mirror, Delphi and Pan were eager to check it out. They touched its surface with their beaks and aimed a lot of echolocation signals at it. It must have been very odd for them to see what appeared to be three-dimensional objects in front of them, their reflections in the mirror, while getting a sonar signal of an essentially flat surface. But if they were surprised, it wasn't obvious to me. They quickly began to interact with the reflections in the mirror as if they were other dolphins. Very soon, though, they moved into what had every appearance of contingency testing. While staring at themselves, they circled and cocked their heads, rocked their bodies back and forth, and opened and closed their mouths. Delphi and Pan definitely appeared to be testing the consequences of their actions, just as Gallup's chimps had done. So far, so good.

On the eighth day of one-hour-per-day exposure to the mirror, both Delphi and Pan moved to the next level. They appeared to engage in self-directed behavior. For instance, they each got very close to the mirror, putting an eye just a few inches away, staring intently. They opened their mouths very wide, as if examining the inside, and then wiggled their tongues, something chimps did with mirrors. At different times, Delphi and Pan each positioned himself so he could see his own ventral surface and then protruded his penis, an action that is under voluntary control in dolphins. And of course there was the courtship dance and reciprocal intromission attempts with each other that I described earlier.

It was all suggestive evidence of self-recognition. But a skeptic could argue that the penis display, and even the intromission attempts, might be a sexual demonstration for the "other" dolphin in the mirror. The skeptic could also argue that the open-mouth behaviors were really open-mouth threats, something dolphins do during aggressive encounters. But this open-mouth behavior was very different; their mouths were relaxed and then held open in a prolonged manner. Also, the penis display had occurred when no other social behavior was directed toward the mirror. As for the intromission attempts, we noticed that when the mirror was covered and unavailable, Delphi and Pan did it in many different locations in the pool, but when the mirror was in plain view, they did it
only
in front of the mirror, always intently gazing at the reflection. And tongue wiggling, as far as we knew, was not part of any aggressive behavior.

On more than one occasion, Pan spent minutes in front of the mirror bending his head toward it, which gave him a view of his blowhole, and then he produced bubbles and bubble streams along with a variety of sounds. We had seen dolphins blow bubbles before, of course, but Pan was obviously looking at himself in the mirror. Delphi and Pan had pretty clearly gone down the cognitive path pioneered by Gallup's chimps two decades earlier. It was time for the crucial mark (with no hands) test.
*

Our plan was that during a feeding session, we would put a mark on each dolphin's body in a place he could not ordinarily see. We would then give the dolphin a release signal to let him know he could swim away and do as he pleased. We marked one animal at a time. If the dolphin was curious about what had happened during the marking session, we expected he might go to the mirror and visually inspect the mark. Or, if he wasn't curious, we thought he might notice the mark in the mirror while swimming freely and then use the mirror to take a closer look.

The first session did not go well. Delphi was the first to be marked. Lori and I and my research assistants were filming and collecting data from an elevated observation deck twelve feet in the air, adjacent to the pool, and I asked one of the dolphin trainers to mark Delphi. The trainer inadvertently put far too much of the mark (white zinc oxide, the main ingredient in many sunscreens) on Delphi's side. It was a huge, thick smear, and it appeared to really freak Delphi out. He broke station without waiting for the releasesignal and started speed swimming around the pool. I was upset that he seemed so upset, so after a few minutes I shouted to the trainer, "We've got to call him back to station, now! Forget the experiment. We have to get this stuff off him." My own heart was pounding as the trainer placed the bucket of fish at the pool's edge and put her hand in the water, part of the signal to come. Delphi came right over. The trainer gave him the hand signal to lay out, which the dolphins had learned to do for physical exams.

The trainer quickly wiped away the big white mark, and Delphi immediately relaxed. Again without a signal, he broke station and made a beeline for the mirror, orienting that part of his body where the mark had been toward the mirror. He appeared to inspect it closely. He then came back to station. We were thrilled. In that little episode, Delphi gave every indication that he knew the dolphin in the mirror was in fact him, and he had used the mirror to check out something on his body. We seemed to be on the way to that final crucial step in the test.

Unfortunately, the use of the mirror to inspect a part of the body
after
the mark was removed was pretty much the
only
such compelling behavior during the mark (with no hands) test. We conducted a few other mark tests, putting a less freaky amount of zinc oxide on both Delphi and Pan, but neither of them made a beeline for the mirror to check it out as we had expected them to. Only after we removed the marks did they unequivocally race to the mirror and quite deliberately orient their bodies to inspect the area. "Whereas the dolphins did seem interested in using the mirror to examine their bodies after the mark had been removed," we wrote later, "we did not find any other instances of posturing that was unambiguous." We were therefore forced to conclude that the work had been
suggestive
but not
conclusive
of self-recognition. We felt we had been so close. (I had conversations about this work with Ken Marten, a friend and colleague at Sea Life Park Hawaii, shortly after we finished this experiment. He decided to embark on a similar project and produced similarly inconclusive results.
9
) It was very frustrating. In retrospect, I think we probably stopped the work prematurely, partly through force of circumstances: I was pregnant and was planning to spend a year back east at Yale with my husband before returning to Vallejo.

***

In his original chimp mirror self-recognition paper, Gallup had essentially said that the self-directed behavior he'd observed in the chimps he had tested was enough to convince a reasonable person that these animals were self-aware but he felt he needed a "scientifically objective" measure: the mark test. We were now in the same position with the dolphins: we all felt in our guts that dolphins, too, were self-aware, given what we'd seen, but they had just failed Gallup's litmus test. Why?

As I mentioned earlier, human children generally develop self-awareness between the ages of eighteen months and two years. This cognitive capacity resides in the prefrontal cortex and elsewhere and is associated with the ability to show concern for others. "Poor Mommy," a two-year-old might say if Mommy gets a boo-boo. But psychologists know very well that not all of them can do this by age two, not even neurologically normal kids, despite the fact that from a very young age all children are cued almost daily, as in "Hey, Morgan, that's you in the mirror. Don't you look
cute!
" When Morgan sees herself in the mirror when she's a little older, she already knows it's her. So when we insist that in order to demonstrate that a nonhuman animal is self-aware it must pass the mirror test, including the mark test, with no prior cuing, we are actually setting the bar far higher than we do for human children.

Recognizing oneself in the mirror seems like a simple act. You roll out of bed in the morning, you go to the mirror, you're either happy or not so happy with what you see, but you know without any effort what you are seeing, and you make use of the mirror to remove that eyelash resting on your cheek or to iron out that wrinkle inflicted by the pillow. Sounds simple, but in fact, cognitively, it is quite complex. In the mirror test, an animal must first pay selective attention to the information in the mirror. Many animals don't do this. Second, if the animal does pay attention, it has to interpret what it sees. Most animals that do pay attention to the mirror interpret the image as another member of their own species and try to engage it in some form of social interaction. If the rare observing animal recognizes the image as "self," it must then be motivated to use the mirror as a tool to observe and inspect itself before we can be sure what it knows. So, self-directed behavior requires both the
cognitive capacity
that underlies the concept of self and the
motivation
to use the mirror as a tool. Passing the mark test is yet more specific: it requires motivation or interest in touching the mark. I see that as a distinct barrier.

Chimps spend a good deal of their time grooming themselves and even more time grooming one another; dolphins, for obvious reasons, do not. Dolphins engage in high degrees of tactile and physical contact with others, both dolphins and humans, and their skin is highly innervated so they are sensitive to touch. But for the most part, paying attention to marks on their body is not high on their daily agendas. Chimps need to pick out lice and other bugs. Dolphins do not. Perhaps that's why Delphi and Pan failed the mark (with no hands) test—perhaps neither one was sufficiently engaged by a foreign mark on his body. In which case, one could argue that the mark test was just unrealistically demanding. Or perhaps our experimental design was in some way inadequate.

In any case, I knew that before long I would have to try again.

6. Through the Looking Glass

T
HE DANCER IN ME
was quite captivated as I watched Presley perform a bizarre sequence of horizontal swirls. He was lying below the water's surface on his left side, his body curled in a fetal position as he spun and looked, spun and looked, spun and looked. From modern dance lessons I had taken as a child, I knew that when a dancer executes a spin, he has to visually fix on a particular point after each rotation. It helps keep the dancer oriented and stable, and during practice sessions in front of a mirror it allows him to check out the aesthetics of the move. Presley appeared to be doing something very similar as he visually fixed on a particular point after each rotation. Spin and look, spin and look, spin and look. Round and round and round he went, this thirteen-year-old male bottlenose dolphin. In the five years I had known him, I'd never seen him (or any other dolphin, for that matter) do this, and he'd not been trained to do it either. It wasn't part of the natural behavioral repertoire of dolphins. Yet Presley was suddenly motivated to carry out the swirl. He was regarding himself in a three-by-five-foot horizontal mirror that I had placed in his pool. This dolphin spin-dancer glanced toward the mirror at the same instant in each rotation, looking at himself.
*

This was early in 1998 at the Wildlife Conservation Society's New York Aquarium in Brooklyn, and I had embarked on mirror-self-recognition investigations again, with two male dolphins, thirteen-year-old Presley and seventeen-year-old Tab, both of them captive-born. I had spent a lot of time thinking about the mirror we should use, trying to put myself in the mind of a dolphin. What would be the best possible demonstration that they really wanted to see themselves in the mirror? Then it came to me one day:
Make it smaller.
This way, if the dolphins truly wanted to see themselves, their actions would have to be quite deliberate. I thought we might see much more specific behavior as an indicator of their intentions. This approach would allow them to show me what they were capable of without my shepherding them toward a particular behavior.

In some ways it makes little difference what size a mirror is if all you are going to do is put an eye close to it, as I'd seen Presley and Tab do separately several times. But if Presley wanted to see himself fully in the three-by-five mirror while he swirled, he'd have to position himself some distance away from it, which is precisely what he did. He quite deliberately backed away from the mirror until he could see his entire body, and then he went into the horizontal swirl: spin and look, spin and look, spin and look. Presley apparently first tested out the physics of seeing the whole of his body in the small mirror, and then went into this entirely novel behavior, a move he invented.

***

Embarking on an ambitious research program with dolphins at the New York Aquarium hadn't been part of my plans when I left Marine World in Vallejo in 1990 for what I envisaged as a year's leave of absence for the birth of my daughter. My husband had a research position at Yale Medical School and we decided it would be best for me to join him there; we planned to stay there for a year and then return to the West Coast, where he would look for a professorship in the Bay Area while I would reunite with the dolphins and resume my position as director of the marine mammal lab. But it turned out to be a tumultuous year for the facility, and a fateful one for me. The administration changed. Amusement rides and other abominations were to be installed. Circe, to my piercing sorrow, was sold to an aquarium in Portugal, and Delphi and Pan were sold to an aquarium in Florida. The new director had decided he wanted to use the research pools as a breeding facility. Apparently, the new regime thought that Delphi and Pan didn't look enough like the prototypical Flipper and so were not suitable for dolphin shows. It's true that the boys were bigger than purebred Atlantic bottlenose dolphins, since they were a cross between the Atlantic and Pacific species. (Delphi's mother, Circe, and Pan's mother, Terry, were Atlantic dolphins, but the boys' father was the lovable, larger Pacific bottlenose Gordo.) But I thought they were adorable, and smart as hell.

BOOK: The Dolphin in the Mirror
9.74Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Like Father Like Daughter by Christina Morgan
Recapitulation by Wallace Stegner
The Paper Cowboy by Kristin Levine
Throttle (Kindle Single) by Hill, Joe, King, Stephen
Halfway Perfect by Julie Cross
Redemption by Howard Fast
Burning Up Flint by Laurann Dohner
Gut Instinct by Linda Mather
Fire Me Up by Kimberly Kincaid