The Proteus Paradox (18 page)

BOOK: The Proteus Paradox
9.58Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

You don't need virtual worlds to weave elaborate personas. Our obsession with pointing out dishonesty online ignores the deception and superficiality that occur offline. And one first fundamental truth of virtual worlds is simply that the people you meet online live in the physical world.

In fairy tales like “Snow White” or “Cinderella,” the all-good fairy godmothers and the all-evil stepmothers aren't merely one-dimensional caricatures of human intentions. This storytelling device serves a powerful function for children. Child psychologist Bruno
Bettelheim argues that these characters create a coping mechanism for children to deal with their own mothers when they get angry or become scary. Because children have a hard time making sense of how someone so loving can suddenly become so scary, this splitting mechanism allows them to keep the kind-mother separate from the scary-mother.
10

We have been telling ourselves a modern-day fairy tale about truth and falsehood simplistically partitioned into two worlds. Too often, media stories about the Internet revolve around the myth that truth and honesty reside in the physical world while fantasy and deception reside in the online world. This is the unspoken logic underlying the sensationalist stories of people finding love online—the drama comes from the tacit question of how people can find true love in a world of falsehoods. By splitting realities, we ignore the fact that honesty and deception are very much a part of both the physical and virtual worlds. As we've seen, people can be quite superficial in the real world. We often assume that fantasy avatars hide the truth, but fantasy avatars can actually reveal the truth. For some people, fantasy worlds allow them to form genuine relationships that couldn't have started in the physical world.

These findings about how people fall in love in online games also reveal an important aspect about relationships in general. Online dating websites often tout their scientific matchmaking methods, many of which rely on long surveys of personality traits and attitudes. The underlying premise is that a lasting relationship can be predicted with an extensive checklist of either similar or complementary traits. This claim taps into a fairy-tale fantasy: there is a perfect prince or princess out there waiting for you if you search hard enough. Unfortunately, there is no empirical evidence to back up these matchmaking methods. A recent review of the psychological
literature has shown that similarities between a couple's personalities account for roughly 0.5 percent of their relationship satisfaction. There is even less evidence that complementarity leads to happy relationships. It turns out that relationships are more than just checklists. Rather than being tools for sophisticated matchmaking, online dating sites may work simply because they provide a roster of single people in your area who are receptive to dating.

The player narratives in this chapter provide a very different perspective of how relationships work. Players developed relationships after going through adventures together and sharing in both disappointments and accomplishments. Relationships are created when two people are willing to work through life's ups and downs together, whether online or offline and regardless of what their checklists look like. Lasting relationships are forged, not found. And the truth is that relationships are a lot of work. This is why online dating websites are so seductive. They are selling the myth that compatible relationships can be deduced with a magical database and require no real work on your part. For both good and bad, online games turn out to be a lot of work. And lasting relationships happen in online games because the gameplay often forces people to build trust and work together. These games jumpstart that forging process.
11

CHAPTER 8 TOOLS OF PERSUASION AND CONTROL

In the last five chapters, I've described how offline categories—such as our ethnicity or gender—find a way to contaminate the assumed utopias of virtual worlds. But the Proteus Paradox isn't just about how our offline politics don't change when we enter virtual worlds, it's also about how things beyond our control end up altering how we think and behave. In this and the remaining chapters, I'll detail how virtual worlds provide unparalleled tools for controlling us.

We're all liars. We're trained to lie to each other in very specific ways so that we immediately forgive each other. And the aisles of supermarkets and drugstores supply us with a vast arsenal of these tools of deception. There are toothpastes and peroxide strips to make your teeth whiter. There are hair-coloring products to cover your grays, and lotions and powders to smooth your complexion. And there are pushup bras and compression shirts to enhance your appearance. As a society, we not only tolerate these modest and temporary self-transformations, we encourage them. No one will get mad at you because your teeth are a little whiter. By conforming to these ideals of appearance, we create a favorable impression on people we meet and interact with. We call it “dressing to impress” or “putting
your best face forward.” But what does it mean to put your best face forward in a virtual world in which you can swap faces at the click of a mouse button? Virtual worlds like
Second Life
feature an extreme form of customization; as the company's product factsheet states, “Using over 150 unique sliders, change everything from your foot size to your eye color to the cut of your shirt.” What are the consequences of being in a world in which facial reconstructive surgery is neither expensive nor painful but cheap and instantaneous?
1

Stealing Faces

When you play a video game on a screen with a resolution of 1900 × 1200 pixels, your eyes receive over 2 million pixels every second. Amazingly, not only do our brains process that information continuously, but gamers can react and respond with lightning reflexes. To accomplish this feat, our brains highlight and prioritize the processing of patterns or points of interest. Faces are a good example of these privileged patterns. Even babies, ten minutes after being born, pay more attention to images that resemble a face than scrambled versions of those same images. Their brains quickly pick out any visual patterns that resemble an oval with two eyes, a nose, and a mouth placed appropriately.
2

Our brains employ many other heuristics to sort through the information deluge of everyday life almost instantaneously. After we pick out faces in this visual data stream, we next need to decide how to react to those faces, the bodies they are attached to, and the gestures their hands are producing. When we meet a stranger at a social gathering, we don't pause the conversation for a minute to sort through this information consciously and then weigh the pros and cons of whether this is someone we want to meet. Instead, a gut
feeling drives our interactions with this stranger. One cue that our brains use to make these snap judgments is how similar the other person is to us. We perceive people who look like us or believe in the same things we do as more attractive and more persuasive than people with whom we share little.
3

Our brains don't have much time to make these decisions, so it turns out that even entirely arbitrary points of similarity can make us like another person more. In a study conducted by psychologist Jerry Burger and his colleagues, undergraduate students met another student taking part in the study—actually a research assistant posing as a participant (a “confederate” in experimental design jargon). During the study, half of the students discovered that they “coincidentally” shared the same birthday with the confederate. The other half, the control group, did not share the same birthday. After the study ostensibly ended, the confederate asked each student if, as part of a class assignment, he or she would mind reading an eight-page essay and providing a one-page critique in just twenty-four hours. The students who thought that they shared the same birthday with the confederate were almost twice as likely to agree to this burdensome request than were those in the control group. Using the same study framework, Burger and his colleagues found that the same effect could be elicited if the students were led to believe that they shared the same first name or fingerprint patterns with the confederate.
4

If faces are given priority treatment by our brains, and similarity leads to attraction and cooperation, could we leverage the extreme customization of virtual worlds, not to create new faces, but to create strangers that look like you? Instead of stealing your birthday or your first name, these virtual strangers could literally steal your face. Of course, this transformation is impossible in the physical world. You could dye your hair or wear colored contacts, but there is no easy
way to change your facial bone structure to match another person's face. In the digital world, however, this is an easy problem to solve. Commercially available software allows you to blend two digital photographs by indicating points of reference—the corners of the eye, the hairline, and so on. This makes it possible to create a virtual stranger whose face shares a precise 20 percent or 40 percent resemblance to your face.

On the eve of the 2004 presidential election between George W. Bush and John Kerry, my graduate adviser, Jeremy Bailenson, was chatting with another professor in the communication department with a background in political science, Shanto Iyengar, about face morphing technology. Because people are more attracted to and more likely to help someone who looks like them, they wondered if they could influence how people voted in an election by blending people's faces into well-known political candidates. In short, if a political candidate looked a little more like you, would you be more likely to vote for him or her? Another graduate student, Nathan Collins, and I were brought onboard the project. Even though past studies suggested that similarity cues increased attraction and persuasion, it wasn't at all clear that it would matter in a high-stakes situation like a presidential election in which people had strong feelings about the candidates. And given how often people had seen posters and television ads of both candidates, it also wasn't clear whether we could tamper with their faces at all without people noticing immediately.

We decided to hedge our bets. We would use a conservative 20 percent face morph for half the study sample and a higher-risk 40 percent morph for the other half. The 160 study participants were a nationally representative sample of voting-age US citizens recruited by Knowledge Networks, a company that offers free web TV in exchange for regular participation in online surveys. The participants
were first asked to provide a digital photograph of themselves to allow us to create the morphed candidate images. Then, one week before the presidential election and ostensibly as an entirely separate study, the same participants were shown a photograph of Bush and a photograph of Kerry and asked about their opinions of the candidates and for whom they were likely to vote. Each participant saw one candidate morphed with their face and the other candidate morphed with someone else (to control for any extraneous effects of digital morphing). We found that face morphing made no difference to strong partisans, but weak partisans and independents were swayed by it. Overall, the effect was significant: among the group of participants morphed with Kerry, Kerry received enough votes to win the presidential election.
5

This study was one of four studies we conducted using face morphing. Altogether, more than six hundred participants were shown digitally morphed photographs blending their faces into political candidates, ranging from 20 percent to 40 percent morphs. In each study, every participant was asked to guess the goal of the experiment. Roughly 3 percent of participants believed that someone had manipulated the candidate's image. Not one participant across the four studies suggested the possibility that his or her own photograph had been blended with the political candidate's. In virtual worlds, tools of persuasion can be powerful yet undetectable.
6

Breaking Reality

This strange new world of face stealing was where I found myself in 2004, a year into my graduate program at Stanford. For four years, I worked with Jeremy Bailenson in the Virtual Human Interaction Lab on dozens of studies exploring one question: What if you could
break reality? This question is rooted in a peculiar feature of virtual worlds. Virtual worlds do such a good job of creating the illusion of shared reality that you have to take a step back to remember that a computer system mediates all the interactions between users. Everything that two people say to each other has to first pass through this digital intermediary. In a world in which two people never have direct contact with each other, the messenger is king. And if you are in control of the virtual world, the messenger is working for you, and thus you become the king.

The implications are far-reaching. In the most simplistic scenario, imagine that the system performs basic sentence parsing and makes sure that all requests contain either the word
please
or a smiley face. If it doesn't, the system simply injects one or the other. Player A is unaware of the injection, and Player B responds more positively than he or she might have, starting off a positive feedback loop. Or these superpowers could be the king's privilege. The king could have a dozen automated helpers injecting mundane pleasantries as needed. The king is always smiling, always saying, “Thank you,” and always remembers the names of your spouse, children, and favorite sports team.

But the king's powers go well beyond that. Consider eye gaze. We know that students feel more engaged and learn better if the teacher pays them more attention. In a virtual classroom, each student sees the shared reality of the virtual world only via his or her own individual display device. This means that these many slices of shared reality do not need to line up with each other. Thus, the teacher in a virtual classroom can maintain eye contact with every single student at the same time. Each student would feel that he or she has the undivided attention of the teacher. In fact, in a virtual classroom, you can warp space itself. In a physical classroom, only one student can sit in the front center of the classroom. In a virtual classroom, there is no
reason why every student cannot sit in the front center of the classroom, each perceiving the other students dynamically repositioned around him or her.

BOOK: The Proteus Paradox
9.58Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Now I Know by Aidan Chambers
Memoria del fuego II by Eduardo Galeano
The Crush by Sandra Brown
James Acton 03 - Broken Dove by J Robert Kennedy
Paranormalcy by Kiersten White
The Curse of the Pharaohs by Elizabeth Peters
The Midnight Man by Paul Doherty