Read Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness Online
Authors: Fabrizio Didonna,Jon Kabat-Zinn
Tags: #Science, #Physics, #Crystallography, #Chemistry, #Inorganic
has evolved different types of affect-behaviour regulation systems
(Panskepp,
1998).
These systems coordinate attention, thoughts, emotions and actions.
One way to conceptualise these affect regulations is as basic systems (Depue
& Morrone-Strupinsky,
2005).
These are as follows: (1) threat-protection system, (2) drive, seeking, and reward system and (3) a contentment-soothing
system. These systems are in constant states of co-regulation and are shown
in Figure
6.1
Various other sub-divisions have been suggested and described
(Panskepp,
1998),
but the three-systems approach offers a useful heuristic for
compassion-focused therapy
(Gilbert,
2005,
2007a,b).
Looked at this way, our threat system can be seen as having certain defensive emotions (e.g.
anger, anxiety, and disgust), a range of behaviour options (e.g. fight, flight,
freeze, and submission;
Marks, 1987),
and various ‘better safe than sorry’
attentional and processing biases
(Gilbert, 1998).
There are also clear
physiological systems that underpin the threat system
(LeDoux, 1998).
Once activated, it creates various physiological patterns in the body underpinning
felt experiences, directs thinking and actions tendencies. The drive system
on the other hand orientates us to things that are rewarding (e.g. food, sex,
money, and status). It is associated with the activated affects of excitement-
linked positive affects. In contrast, the ‘contentment system’ enables animals
99
100
Paul Gilbert and Dennis Tirch
Types of Affect Regulation Systems
Content, safe, connect
Drive, excite, vitality
Affiliative focused
Incentive/resource
focused
Soothing/safeness
Seeking and behaviour
activating
Opiates (?)
Dopamine (?)
Threat-focused
safety seeking
Activating/inhibiting
Serotonin (?)
Anger, anxiety disgust,
Figure. 6.1.
Types of affect regulation systems.
to be quiescent when they no longer need to acquire resources and are
not under any threat. This system appears to be associated with a sense of
(soothing) peaceful well-being. During evolution the contentment system
has evolved into a soothing system that can be triggered by social stimuli of
affection, love and care
(Carter, 1998; Depue & Morrone-Strupinsky
,
2005).
The development, coordination and co-regulation of these three basic
systems are dependent on gene-learning interactions. Indeed, biological
organisms are designed to be changed and moulded by life experiences. Dif-
ferent experiences encourage and strengthen some neuronal connections
and weaken others
(LeDoux, 2002).
For example, it is now known that
harsh, neglectful and/or abusive backgrounds have major impacts on the
maturing brain of young children, especially on those areas that regulate emo-
tions such as connections between the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and amygdala
(Cozolino, 2007;
Schore, 1994;
Siegel, 2001).
Life experiences are coded as
emotional memories
, linked to synaptic sensitisation at one level–through
to the complex brain systems dedicated to different types and forms of mem-
ory – such as episodic, semantic, and short- and long-term memory
(LeDoux,
Understanding the way life experiences shape the brain’s various sensi-
tivities in threat and positive affect systems, and emotional memories, is
important because we know that emotional disorders are linked to early
affect sensitisation and emotional memories. Indeed, some therapists place
the activation of emotional memories, at both implicit and explicit levels,
centre stage to psychopathology
(Brewin, 2006).
Psychodynamic (Greenberg
& Mitchell, 1983) and behavioural theorists
(Ferster, 1973)
have long argued that emotional memories, associations and conditioning need not be conscious but still highly influential on how people process and respond to life
events and situations.
Most people who experience psychological problems, that require some
kind of intervention, feel under threat from various aspects of their lives
(e.g. in social relationships) or their inner experiences (e.g. being over-
whelmed by emotions or memories or negative, ruminative thoughts). Thus,
Chapter 6 Emotional Memory, Mindfulness and Compassion
101
depression, anxiety, paranoia, eating disorder, phobias, PTSD and OCD are
all related to threat-focused processing and efforts to regulate threat and get
safe. Hence, most psychological therapies aim to help people recognise the
early and current sources of heightened threat and loss sensitivities, vari-
ous thoughts and feelings that automatically ‘jump into mind’, their ways
of processing threats/losses from memory
(Brewin, 2006),
schematic representations of self and others
(Beck, Freeman, Davis et al., 2003)
and coping strategies (e.g. vigilance and avoidance). Through various interventions that
may involve the therapeutic relationship, exposure, cognitive and emotional
change, and new behaviour strategies, therapies try to reduce threat/loss
sensitivities and threat/loss processing. In this way the external and internal
stimuli that have activated threat/loss processing systems lose their power to
do so.
One aspect that increases threat sensitivity and focuses threat processing is
our human
meta-cognitive
abilities
(Wells, 2000).
These have given us huge advantages in being able to plan, anticipate and cooperate and are the source
of culture, civilisation and science – but these abilities come with a cost.
Chimpanzees probably do not worry that the pain in the chest could be a
heart attack, or if they eat too much they will get fat and, in some social
groups, might be rejected; they do not worry about their future prospects in
family or work. Humans, however, live in both a world of ‘is’ (linked to direct
sensory experiences) and one of ‘imagination and meta-cognitions’ where
we can focus on the past and future, the feared, the lost and the hoped for
(Gilbert, 2007a; Singer, 2006).
We can construct plans and scenarios in our minds and then respond to them like real stimuli
(Wells, 2000).
Our imaginations are not physiologically neutral; rather fantasies (e.g. sexual) can stimu-
late physiological systems and produce arousal (e.g. sexual fantasies). When
our attention is absorbed in this inner world of thinking, imagining or being
overwhelmed by emotional memories, we are no longer open to live ‘in’ the
present moment. We are dragged away from ‘the present moment’ because
other systems in our brains are pulling on the field of consciousness demand-
ing attention. For example, different emotional memories and conditioning
means that we react quickly to things – our bodies might start reacting to a
situation before we are consciously aware of it, and then our emotions rush
us along, focusing our thoughts and behaviours.
Mindfulness
Mindfulness addresses both these problems. We can learn to be attentive
to emotions and thoughts as they are triggered, to see them as linked to
emotional memories and conditioning. We switch to an ‘observer’ mode –
being able to notice and describe what happens inside us rather than be cap-
tured by it. Many therapies help people switch to this observing-describing
mode of attention. Mindfulness also helps us to become more aware of the
way our minds wander from the present moment into daydreams, the past
and future, and with regret, anticipation or apprehension. By noticing the
way consciousness is ‘grabbed by these inner concerns’ and our emotions
affected, we are enabled to pull the attention back and thus reduce and calm
102
Paul Gilbert and Dennis Tirch
the feedback loops between threat arousal and the maintaining effects of
certain meta-cognitions and ruminations.
For over two and half thousand years Buddhist psychology has seen human
psychology as dominated by the efforts of our minds to cope with the
inevitability of threats, losses and harms that give rise to suffering; none of
us are immune to life’s frustrations, adversities and final decay and death of
ourselves and of those we love. At the heart of the Buddhist approach is to
train our minds in ways that enable us to ‘face’ but also ‘flow with’ the harsh
realties of life. The two most important tasks in this mind training are those
of mindfulness and compassion.
In the last 20 years mindfulness has attracted considerable attention as
both a way to promote well-being and also a therapeutic process for specific
difficulties (e.g. recurrent depression). For the most part these approaches
focus on how to train one’s attention so that we learn to pay attention to the
present moment without judgement. Hence mindfulness is a mode of expe-
riencing and is suggested to be a fundamental psychological state involved
in the alleviation of suffering
(Corrigan, 2004;
Martin, 1997;
Fulton & Seigel,
2005).
The contemporary protégé Tibetan meditation master Yongyey
Mingyur Rinpoche (2007)
described mindfulness as ‘the key, the
how
of Buddhist practice [that] lies in learning to simply rest in a bare awareness of
thoughts, feelings and perceptions as they occur.’
Humans depend on verbal-linguistic and logico-mathematical processing
in a great deal of their interactions with the environment. However, as
noted above, the dominance of these processing mechanisms in human con-
scious experience can result in a disconnection from moment-to-moment
experience and a reification and concretisation of internal emotional experi-
ences
(Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001; Hayes, Stroshal, Wilson, 1999).
Because humans have meta-cognitive abilities, we can plan (what will hap-
pen if I do X; how can I get Y) and be fearful (what will happen if X hap-
pens) – all purely on the basis of our thoughts, attributions, exceptions and
anticipations. As a result, humans may often spend their time responding to
internal thoughts, predictions and intrusive memories as though they are real
events. This ‘literalisation’ of mental representations has been referred to as
‘cognitive fusion’
(Hayes et al., 1999).
The distress that may arise in the presence of painful, literalised cogni-
tions and emotional memories is clear and obvious. However, the way people
try to cope with emotional sensitivities, intrusions, ruminations and mem-
ories – that pull on their thought processes – may be even more signifi-
cant. For example, research has demonstrated that attempts at thought sup-
pression or avoidance (both common coping strategies) often serve only to
increase the frequency, pull and intrusiveness of painful thoughts, feelings
and predictions
(Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996;
Wegner, Schneider, Knutson, & McMahon,
1991).
Under such conditions, our emotional memories, associational learning patterns, and the nature of human
relational responding create a paradoxical prison, wherein our attempts to
reject and ignore painful experiences only serve to drag our attention back
to the internal constructs which drive our suffering. Buddhist psychology
describes such a phenomenon as
Samsara
(cyclic existence), a cycle of
persistently re-experiencing (i.e. re-incarnating) our suffering through grasp-
ing at what we cannot have and rejecting that which we do not wish to
Chapter 6 Emotional Memory, Mindfulness and Compassion
103
experience. Historically, this re-experiencing was construed as a returning
to lives of suffering after death. However, a post-modern, 21st-century, West-
ern perspective may interpret this
Samsara
as a remarkably apt and concise
description of a life spent experientially fused with emotional memories and
dysfunctional cognitions. Mindfulness is a way of recognising the eruptions
in thoughts and feelings, the pull and flow in our thinking that link to our per-
sonal sensitivities. It also trains the mind to be with them but not ‘in’ them.
We noted above that many of our personal threat sensitivities and the links
between thoughts and emotions can be understood as emergent for the inter-
play of various neurophysiological systems. Mindfulness research has focused
on its neurophysiological effects. In fact, research across a range of levels of
analysis, from neuroimaging to clinical outcome studies, has demonstrated
the effectiveness of mindfulness-based practice in helping people change
their relationship to their emotions. Recent experimental research has found
that a 15-minute focused breathing induction, which parallels aspects of
mindfulness training, resulted in greater capacity for emotion regulation and