Read The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language Online
Authors: Steven Pinker
H
ERE ARE THE QUESTIONS
I get asked the most often in connection with
The Language Instinct
:
Did you ever study or work with Noam Chomsky?
No. He is a linguist, and I trained as an experimental psychologist. During the twenty-one years I taught at MIT, we were in different departments, and given how universities work, our paths didn’t cross much from day to day. Our relationship is cordial, and he has deeply influenced my views on language, but our approaches also differ in important ways.
Do your academic colleagues resent your crossover success?
Not that I can tell. Both at MIT and at Harvard, I have received nothing but kind support. Elsewhere, many academics have thanked me for keeping their students awake or for explaining to their parents what they do for a living. Of course, many express their disagreements with me vociferously, as they do (and ought to do) with any scholar with strong views. But academia is not nearly as petty as many outsiders assume.
How many languages do you speak?
I have some acquaintance with French, Hebrew, and Spanish, but am far from fluent and never rely on this knowledge when I write about language. When my research involves a foreign language, I collaborate with someone who is both a native speaker and an expert in the linguistics literature. In my general writings I occupy a high link in the linguistics food chain, relying on surveys of the world’s languages by experts on linguistic diversity, who in turn rely on experts in particular languages.
Do you still do empirical research?
Yes. Since I wrote
The Language Instinct
, I have studied past tense and plural inflection in children (including identical and fraternal twins), in neurological patients, in speakers of English, Hebrew, and German, and with the use of functional MRI and other neuroimaging techniques. Recently I have shifted my focus to semantics and pragmatics, pursuing questions that grew out of
The Stuff of Thought
.
My spouse speaks a language other than English (or We’re spending a year in a foreign country). Is there anything I can do to encourage my children to retain their second language as they grow up?
Children care more about their peers than about their parents, so send them to summer camps, after-school programs, or vacations with their cousins, where they will have to use the language with kids their own age.
My child is three years old and hasn’t said a word. What should I do?
Take him (it’s more likely to be a “him”) to a speech and language pathologist accredited by the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (www.asha.org), ideally one affiliated with a university, clinic, or teaching hospital. If the child seems to understand language, and is bright and socially responsive, there is a good chance that he is simply a “late talker” (probably from genetic causes) who will outgrow his silence and end up just fine. (See also the recommendations in “Read on”)
M
ANY FIELDS OF SCIENCE
enjoy a rapid rate of new discoveries and a high degree of consensus about what they mean. The study of language, unfortunately for linguistics but perhaps fortunately for
The Language Instinct
, is not among them. A book on human genetics or nanotechnology would be hopelessly out of date a dozen years after its publication, but I like to think that
The Language Instinct
is still a useful introduction to the science of language today. Of course the field has hardly stood still (nor have my opinions), and here are some reflections on the contents of each chapter in the light of developments since 1994.
Chapter 1: An Instinct to Acquire an Art
. The two heroes of this chapter are very much in the spotlight today. Darwin’s influence has expanded in psychology, the social sciences, philosophy, medicine, and genomics (despite the retrograde efforts of the “Intelligent Design” movement), and my 2002 book,
The Blank Slate
, explored some of the ramifications of his influence. Chomsky is still the most influential living linguist, and his political writings have inspired a new generation of leftists (I recently saw a sticker on a lamppost that said, “Read Chomsky.”)
Many readers concluded from this chapter’s acknowledgment of Chomsky’s influence on linguistics that I am a “Chomskyan.” In some regards—the view that language comes from a mental system tailored for the computation of symbolic representations—I am. But in the chapter I hinted at some disagreements, and I have spelled them out in the years since. In
The Blank Slate
I explained why I don’t share Chomsky’s romantic view of human nature or the radical leftist-anarchist politics connected to it, and he and I have locked horns in a recent debate on grammatical theory and the evolution of language (more on this later). My views on language and mind are very close to those of my comrade in that debate, the linguist Ray Jackendoff, himself a former student of Chomsky’s. Jackendoff lays out a coherent vision for the science of language in his recent book
Foundations of Language
, which I endorse wholeheartedly.
Chapter 2: Chatterboxes
. Many of the phenomena discussed in this natural history of language have made the news in the past dozen years:
Chapter 3: Mentalese
. When I wrote this chapter, the Whorfian hypothesis was largely out of favor among linguists and psychologists, but the pendulum has swung back, and there is now a lively neo-Whorfian movement. In
The Stuff of Thought
, I review this new research and argue that the problem with the idea that language affects thought is not that it’s entirely wrong but that there are many
ways
in which language can affect thought, and people tend to blur them together. In particular, people tend to confuse banal observations, such as that one person’s words can affect another person’s thoughts (if that weren’t true, language as a whole would be useless) with radical claims, such as that we think in our native language, and that the language we speak makes it impossible to think certain thoughts. In the new book I argue that the major conclusion in “Mentalese” is right—we think not in our native language but in more abstract media of thought.
Chapter 4: How Language Works
. The machinery of syntax described at the end of this chapter is barely recognizable in Chomsky’s current theory, which he calls the “Minimalist Program.” Chomsky is famous within linguistics for overturning his theory every decade or so; the current release, depending on how you count, is 5.2, whereas the release I presented in this chapter was a stripped-down version of 3.2, the “Revised Extended Standard Theory.” Nonetheless, the picture of grammar presented here will be recognizable to anyone reading on in linguistics, because I pretty much stuck to features that would stand the test of time and translate easily to other theories. In my own work I have always favored theories with less exotic machinery than Chomsky’s (shallower trees, fewer traces, less movement), and whose structures are more visible at the surface, like phrases, lexical items, and constructions. Joan Bresnan’s theory is an example, and a more recent version with this flavor can be found in Ray Jackendoff and Peter Culicover’s
Simpler Syntax
.
The most stunning development since I wrote the chapter took place in 2004, when the Boston Red Sox won the World Series.
Chapter 5: Words, Words, Words
. In two subsequent books, I have explored the world of words in far greater depth.
Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language
looks at the combinatorial richness of word formation and its implications for the machinery of cognition.
The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature
looks at the meanings of words and how they originate and spread.
In his lovely book
How Children Learn the Meanings of Words
, Paul Bloom argues that children have no mental mechanisms dedicated to word learning and learn a word the way they learn any other kind of fact. Children zero in on a word’s meaning by exercising their “theory of mind” or intuitive psychology, deducing what a sensible speaker is probably referring to in the context. Ray Jackendoff and I think this is not the whole story, for reasons we explained in our paper debating Chomsky.
Chapter 6: The Sounds of Silence
. Speech recognition technology has advanced tremendously and is now inescapable in telephone information systems. But as everyone who has been trapped in “voice-mail jail” knows, the systems are far from foolproof (“I’m sorry, but I did not understand what you said”). And here is how the novelist Richard Powers described his recent experience with a state-of-the-art speech recognition program: “This machine is a master of speakos and mondegreens. Just as we might hear the…Psalms avow that ‘Shirley, good Mrs. Murphy, shall follow me all the days of my life,’ my tablet has changed ‘book tour’ to ‘back to work’ and ‘I truly couldn’t see’ to ‘a cruelly good emcee.’” Recognizing a large number of words from a large number of speakers is still a formidable engineering task.
The sound pattern of English and the logic behind the vagaries of its spelling are explored more deeply in
Words and Rules
, including the extraordinary suggestion by Chomsky and Morris Halle that English spelling “comes remarkably close to being an optimal orthographic system.”
One raging public debate involving language went unmentioned in
The Language Instinct
: the “reading wars,” or dispute over whether children should be explicitly taught to read by decoding the sounds of words from their spelling (loosely known as “phonics”) or whether they can develop it instinctively by being immersed in a text-rich environment (often called “whole language”). I tipped my hand in the paragraph in this chapter which said that language is an instinct but reading is not. Like most psycholinguists (but apparently unlike many school boards), I think it’s essential for children to be taught to become aware of speech sounds and how they are coded in strings of letters. Diane McGuinness’s
Why Our Children Can’t Read
is my favorite book on the subject. The tide came from the publisher’s marketroids; she originally called it
The Reading Revolution
, since it was about both a scientific revolution in reading research and the revolution in human history that gave us alphabetic writing.