Read Secondary Schizophrenia Online
Authors: Perminder S. Sachdev
[66],
who denied any localization of hallucinations
4. Patients with simple hallucinations may not
to anatomy or any association with the blind field.
report them because they are short lasting,
An important aspect of the anatomy underlying the
regarded as insignificant, or are not even noticed.
occurrence of VH is the set of connections between the
different areas. This hodological feature is discussed at
Within selected groups (mostly of people with
length by ffytche
[96].
visual disease), the estimates vary from 10% to 51%
[67,
100, 101, 102, 103,
104].
Visual impairment (even
total blindness) does not necessarily lead to VHs
[100].
Also, with a progressive disease such as AMD, halluci-
What determines the total duration of
nations may commence long after the initial diagnosis
hallucinations and why do they usually
of the disease
[105].
eventually cease?
Course and outcome
Associated with the degree of complexity of the hallu-CBS hallucinations are usually not unpleasant and
cinations is the total period over which they are man-in virtually all cases eventually disappear. They do
ifest. In general, it is the case that complex hallucina-not normally require treatment although, of course,
tions persist for a long time, sometimes years, whereas
the underlying cause of the hallucinations must be
simple hallucinations last days or weeks. To take again
determined and treated. Patients are generally relieved
the extreme cases of Bonnet’s grandfather and myself,
when they are told that they have no mental illness.
his hallucinations lasted for several months, whereas
CBS has been treated successfully with antiepileptic
mine lasted for 10 to 12 days. Another example of
medication, for example, carbamazepine
[106],
and
a mild deafferentation associated with a short-lasting
with serotonin reuptake inhibitors
[107].
The success-manifestation of VHs is the linkage of these two fea-ful treatment of VH using a steroid in a patient with
tures during macular translocation
[17].
temporal arteritis without effect on the loss of vision
According to the deafferentation theory, a de-
emphasizes the importance of cerebral ischemia in
afferented neuron becomes hyperexcitable and may
the development of VH
[108].
The same conclusion is
develop an increased spontaneous activity. It follows
reached when VH occurs after strokes
[109].
In one
that re-afferentation (reinnervation) should reverse
case CBS resulting from a stroke was completely sup-
this sequence. The nervous system has this universal
pressed by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the
property that denervation or disuse causes sprouting
occipital cortex, an effect not understood but obviously
of nearby neurons
[98].
In the visual system, there
of therapeutic importance
[110].
are several examples of this, for example, a retinal
lesion causes sprouting of cortical neurons in the vicin-ity of the deafferented synapses
[98, 99].
It would be
Implications for clinical practice
expected that sprouting would occur at all deafferented
Correct diagnosis is most important. The first step is
synapses. However, sprouting is a fairly localized pro-to establish that there is no psychiatric condition. The
cess and, if the lesion is extensive, there may be few or
second step is to get some evidence about the likely
no nearby intact afferent neurons. On the other hand,
cause of the hallucinations (visual impairment, drugs,
a small injury would create few deafferented synapses
other diseases). The patient should then be referred to
374
and a speedy recovery could be expected.
the appropriate specialist. Walterfang and colleagues
Chapter 30 – The Charles Bonnet Syndrome
provide a very useful discussion of psychiatric consul-Patients should be encouraged to make careful
tation
[111].
notes and sketches of their hallucinations, especially
if they are of the simple (geometric) type. For
Suggestions for future research
example, an attempt should be made to determine the
size of the hallucination and of its component parts.
Whenever the opportunity presents itself, an imaging
This can be done by superimposing the hallucination
technique should be used at the time of a hallucina-
against a feature in the room, for example, a picture on
tion in order to localize the relevant anatomy. There is
the wall. Three measurements are needed: the dimen-
some evidence that Single Photon Emission Computed
sions of the feature (picture), the fraction occupied by
Tomography (SPECT) may be the most sensitive tech-
the hallucination, and the distance of the patient from
nology
[112].
the feature. From these data, the size of the VH and its
There is evidence that VHs may be an early sign of
component parts (in degrees) can be determined from
dementia
[2,
113, 114, 115].
The occurrence of VHs
trigonometric tables. Size should always be reported
without peripheral visual pathology may indicate a
in degrees because the size in centimeters will vary
central defect not restricted to the visual system. It is
with the distance at which the eyes are focused. The
advisable to keep track of all such cases. In particular,
position of the VH in relation to the point of gaze
if dementia does develop, it should be asked whether
(above, below, left, right) should be noted. These data
the antecedent hallucinations give any predictive clue,
may help to relate the hallucination to an anatomical
for example, were they simple or complex?
feature.
375
Related Concepts – Section 4
References
hallucinations in psychologically
22. Grüsser O. J. Migraine
normal people: Charles Bonnet’s
phosphenes and the retino-
1. Bonnet C. (1760). Essai
syndrome. Lancet, 1996.
347
:
cortical magnification factor.
Analytique sur les facultés de
794–7.
Vision Res, 1995.
35
(8):1125–34.
l’âme. Copenhagen: Philibert.
12. Chen C. S., Lin S. F. Charles
23. Wilkinson F. Auras and other
2. Johnson T. H. Visual
Bonnet syndrome and multiple
hallucinations: windows on the
hallucinations accompanying
sclerosis. Am J Psychiatry, 2001.
visual brain. Prog Brain Res, 2004.
organic lesions of the brain, with
158
:1158–9.
144
:305–20.
special reference to their value as
localizing phenomena. Trans Am
13. Gross N. D., Wilson D. J., Dailey
24. Mascaro J., Formiga F., Pujol R.
Ophthalmol Soc, 1933.
31
:344–94.
R. A. Visual hallucinations after
Charles-Bonnet syndrome
3. de Morsier G. Les automatismes
enucleation. Ophthal Plast
exacerbated by tramadol. Aging
visuels (Hallucinations visuelles
Reconstr Surg, 1997.
13
:221–5.
(Milano), 2003.
15
:518–9.
rétro-chiasmatiques).
14. Gonzalez-Delgado M., Tunin A.,
25. Ben´ıtez del Rosario M. A.,
Schweizerische Medizinische
Salas P. Sindrome de Charles
Montón F., Salinas A.,
et al.
Wochenscrift, 1936.
29
:700–3.
Bonnet. Neurologia, 2004.
Charles Bonnet Syndrome and
4. de Morsier G. Le syndrome de
19
(2):80–2.
opioids. J Am Geriatr Soc, 2001.
Charles Bonnet: hallucinations
49
(2):235–6.
15. Plesnicar B. K., Zolar B., Bocic M.
visuaelles des vieillards sans
B. The Charles Bonnet syndrome:
26. Tan C. S. H., Yong V. K. Y.,
deficience mentale. Ann Med
A case report. Wien Klin
Au Eong K. G. Onset of Charles
Psychol (Paris), 1967.
2
(5):677–
Wochenschr, 2004.
116
(Suppl. 2):
Bonnet syndrome (formed visual
702.
75–7.
hallucinations) following bilateral
5. Podoll K. Osterheider M, Noth J.
laser peripheral iridotomies. Eye,
16. Cohen S. Y., Safran A. B.,
Das Charles Bonnet-Syndrom.
2004.
18
:647–9.
Tadayoni T.,
et al.
Visual
Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr, 1989.
hallucinations immediately after
27. Tomsak R., Zaret C. R.,
57
:43–60.
macular photocoagulation. Am J
Weidenthal D. Charles Bonnet
6. Gold K., Rabins P. V. Isolated
Ophthalmol, 2000.
129
:815–6.
syndrome precipitated by
visual hallucinations and the
17. Au Eong K-G., Fujii G. Y., Ng E.
brimonidine tartrate eye drops.
Charles Bonnet Syndrome: a
W. M.,
et al.
Transient formed
Br J Ophthalmol, 2003.
87
:917.
review of the literature and
visual hallucinations following
28. Teunisse R. J., Zitman F. G., Raes
presentation of six cases. Compr
macular translocation for
D. C. M. Clinical evaluation of 14
Psychiatry, 1989.
30
:90–8.
subfoveal choroidal
patients with the Charles Bonnet
7. Fernandez A., Lichtshein G.,
neovascularization secondary to
Syndrome (Isolated Visual
Vieweg V. R. W. The Charles
age-related macular degeneration.
Hallucinations). Compr
Bonnet Syndrome: a review.
Am J Ophthalmol, 2001.
131
:
Psychiatry, 1994.
35
:70–5.
J Nerv Ment Dis, 1997.
185
(3):
664–6.
29. Alroe C. J., McIntyre J. N. M.
195–200.
18. Lance J. W. Simple formed
Visual hallucinations. The Charles
8. Menon G. J., Rahman I. , Menon
hallucinations confined to the
Bonnet syndrome and
S. J.,
et al.
Complex visual
area of a specific visual field
bereavement. Med J Aust, 1983.
hallucinations in the visually
defect. Brain, 1976.
99
:719–34.
2
:674–5.
impaired: the Charles Bonnet
19. Anderson S. W., Rizzo M.
30. Nicholls D. E. Hallucinogens.
syndrome. Surv Ophthalmol,
Hallucinations following occipital
Pharmacol Ther, 2004.
2003.
48
(1):58–72.
lobe damage: the pathological
101
:131–81.
9. ffytche D. H. Visual hallucinations
activation of visual
31. Gödecke-Koch T., Schlimme T.,
in eye disease. Curr Opin Neurol,
representations. J Clin Exp
Fada D.,
et al.
Charles-
2009.
22
:28–35.
Neuropsychol, 1994.
16
:651–63.
Bonnet-Syndrom bei einer älteren
10. Needham W. E., Taylor R. E.
20. Pomeranz H. D., Lessell S.
Patientin mit beidseitigem
Atypical Charles Bonnet
Palinopsia and polyopia in the
Visusverlust, Hyperthyreose und
hallucinations. An elf in the
absence of drugs or cerebral
relativer Digitalis- ¨
Uberdosierung.
woodshed, a spirit of evil, and the
disease. Neurology, 2000.
Nervenarzt, 2002.
¨
U73
:
cowboy malefactors. J Nerv Ment
54
(4):855–9.
471–4.
Dis, 2000.
188
:108–15.
21. Richards W. The fortification
32. Stofler P. M., Franzoni A. S.,
11. Teunisse R. J., Cruysberg J. R.,
illusions of migraines. Sci Am,
Di˙Fazio I.,
et al.
Charles Bonnet
376
Hoefnagels W. H.,
et al.
Visual
1971.
224
(5):88–96.