Read Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness Online
Authors: Fabrizio Didonna,Jon Kabat-Zinn
Tags: #Science, #Physics, #Crystallography, #Chemistry, #Inorganic
and self-reported associations of emotions with the self.
Brown and Ryan
(2003)
found that, in general, people showed little or no concordance
between explicit (self-) reports of their pleasant and unpleasant emotional
Chapter 4 Phenomenology and Emotional Correlates of Mindfulness
71
self-concept on the one hand and their implicit emotional self-concept on
the other. However, those higher in MAAS-assessed mindfulness showed a
stronger concordance between explicit and implicit emotional self-concept,
suggesting that these individuals may have greater emotional self-awareness.
This finding is consistent with the phenomenological nature of mindfulness
discussed earlier, but the research is still preliminary, and replication and
extension are needed before it can be concluded that mindfulness fosters
emotional awareness.
There is more research addressing the other primary emotion regulatory
skill, namely, the alteration of emotional responses. First, trait mindfulness
and mindfulness practice skills have been associated with less thought sup-
pression, rumination, impulsivity, and passivity, all maladaptive forms of reg-
ulation linked with poorer mental health
(Baer et al., 2006;
Brown & Ryan,
2003;
Cardaciotto et al., 2008;
Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008;
Feldman et al.,
2007;
Frewen et al.,
in press;
McKee et al., 2007;
Shapiro, Brown, & Biegel,
2007;
Wupperman et al., in press). Conversely, mindfulness and mindfulness skills have been positively associated with adaptive regulatory strategies,
including acceptance and letting go of negative thoughts (e.g.,
Baer et al.,
2006;
Brown & Ryan, 2003;
Frewen et al., in press). The adaptive nature of acceptance of emotional and other subjective experiences is consistent with
the notion that it is sometimes more adaptive to experience or express an
emotion than to alter its trajectory
(Barrett & Gross, 2001).
Beyond such preliminary investigations of dispositional emotion regula-
tory tendencies, several trait-based studies have tested the efficacy of mind-
fulness to attenuate the experience of negative emotion in emotionally
provocative situations. Among the most emotionally charged situations that
individuals find themselves in are those involving interpersonal conflict. It
has been argued
(Goleman, 2006)
that the receptive attentiveness that char-
acterizes mindfulness may promote a greater ability or willingness to take
interest in a communication partner’s thoughts and emotions and may also
enhance an ability to attend to the content of a partner’s communication
while also being aware of the partner’s (sometimes subtle) affective tone and
nonverbal behavior. At the same time, such a person may be more aware
of their own cognitive, emotional, and verbal responses to the communica-
tion.
Boorstein (1996)
has argued that mindfulness promotes an ability to
witness thought and emotion so as not to react impulsively and destructively
to them. Initial research guided by this theorizing has been conducted in the
realm of romantic relationships, in which studies have addressed whether
mindfulness may affect the emotional tone of romantic partner conflicts and,
perhaps relatedly, enhance the communication that happens within those
relationships.
Barnes, Brown, Krusemark, Campbell, and Rogge
(2007)
and Wachs and
Cordova
(2007)
found that higher MAAS-measured trait mindfulness pre-
dicted higher relationship satisfaction and greater capacities to respond
constructively to relationship stress among non-distressed dating couples
and married couples. In the second study in their series with dating cou-
ples,
Barnes et al. (2007)
tested the reliability of those findings in the heat of a relationship conflict. Using a well-validated paradigm (e.g., Gottman,
Coan, Carrere, & Swanson,
1998),
higher trait MAAS scores predicted lower emotional stress responses to conflict (anxiety and anger hostility), and this
72
Kirk Warren Brown and Shari Cordon
effect was explained by lower emotional stress before the discussion. This
corroborates other cross-sectional and experience sampling research, noted
already, showing that those more dispositionally mindful are less suscepti-
ble to negative emotional states in general, and suggests that this lower sus-
ceptibility extends into the specific context of romantic couple interactions.
Interestingly, Barnes et al.’s (2007) results showed that rather than buffering
the effects of emotional arousal during conflict, mindfulness helped to inoc-
ulate against such arousal. The capacity of mindfulness to inhibit reactivity
to conflict was also evident in the cognitive judgments that each partner
made; those higher in trait mindfulness showed a more positive (or less neg-
ative) pre–post conflict change in their perception of the partner and the
relationship. This study also supported the importance of bringing a mindful
state into challenging exchanges, in that self-reported state mindfulness was
related to better communication quality, as assessed by objective raters of
the videotaped conflicts.
Whether mindfulness influences affective appraisals has also been tested
experimentally in two studies using state inductions of mindfulness. Arch and
Craske
(2006)
used a focused breathing exercise to induce a mindful state, while two experimental control groups received inductions of unfocused
attention and worrying. Relative to experimental controls, those receiving a
mindfulness induction showed less negative reactivity and emotional volatil-
ity in response to affectively valenced picture slides and a greater willing-
ness to maintain visual contact with aversive slides. This latter finding sug-
gests evidence for one process theorized to explain the salutary effects
of mindfulness on emotion regulation and mental health, namely, willing
exposure to threatening information. Interestingly, this study also found that
those receiving mindfulness instructions maintained consistent, moderately
positive responses to neutral picture slides, while the groups induced by
unfocused attention and worry responded more negatively to neutral slides,
providing some basis for the claim that mindfulness helps to protect against
negatively biased processing of experience.
The
Barnes et al. (2007),
Wachs and Cordova (2007),
and
Arch and Craske
(2006)
findings suggest that mindfulness may influence emotional content by altering situational meaning through a primary appraisal process, in particular by reducing negative emotional reactivity to challenging stimuli. Other
evidence suggests that a mindful state may alter the time course of emo-
tion by facilitating recovery following a provocative event.
Broderick (2005)
found that, in comparison to those in distraction and rumination condi-
tions, individuals in a mindful induction condition showed quicker emotional
recovery from an induced sad mood. Though preliminary, these findings
on reduced reactivity and speeding the recovery from unpleasant emotional
experiences offer support for the hypothesized consequences of the recep-
tive, non-evaluative mode of processing that characterizes mindfulness, and
also offer promise for clinical research by suggesting a means to cope with
difficult emotions when they arise
(Broderick, 2005).
Dynamic relations between mindfulness and emotional content and
regulation
. As other chapters in this volume attest, a growing body of
research indicates that mindfulness-based interventions can have positive
impacts on mental health. Mindfulness interventions are purported to
increase participants’ mindfulness, and this is believed to be responsible for
Chapter 4 Phenomenology and Emotional Correlates of Mindfulness
73
the positive effects of the interventions on cognitive, emotional, and behav-
ioral indicators of mental health. Yet to date little research has examined
whether mindfulness itself is enhanced through such multi-modal treatments
and whether such enhancements are related to emotional content, emotion
regulation, and other mental health outcomes observed. Such research can
not only help to address basic questions about the role of mindfulness in
mental health, but can also inform study of the processes by which mind-
fulness interventions achieve their beneficial effects. In large part, the lack
of attention to such questions is because measures of the mindfulness con-
struct have developed only recently, but since then, intervention studies
have begun to test the dynamic relation between change in mindfulness and
changes in emotional and cognitive indicators of mental health in healthy,
healthy stressed, and clinical populations.
Several uncontrolled studies have shown scores on dispositional mind-
fulness and mindfulness practice skills to increase significantly over the
course of MBSR and related interventions with healthy and distressed sam-
ples (e.g., Carmody & Baer, 2008;
Cohen-Katz et al., 2005;
Frewen et al., in press; Forman, Herbert, Moitra, Yeomans, & Geller, 2007). In a study of
healthy adults participating in a 10-day intensive mindfulness training, Cham-
bers et al. (2008) found that, relative to matched control participants, trained
participants reported significant increases in MAAS mindfulness from pre-
to post-training and significant reductions in negative affect, reflective rumi-
nation, and depressive symptoms. Increases in mindfulness over the study
period were associated with declines in anxiety, depressive symptoms, and
reflective rumination, and increases in positive affect and working memory.
Other intervention studies testing these dynamic associations have focused
on health-care professionals, and professionals in training, whose occupa-
tions can put them at risk for a range of stress-related conditions, including
depression, anxiety, emotional exhaustion, and occupational burnout (Sher-
win et al.,
1992; Tyssen, Vaglum, Gronvold, & Ekeberg, 2001).
In a matched-control MBSR study of psychotherapists in training,
Shapiro et al. (2007)
found that intervention participants reported significant increases in MAAS-
assessed mindfulness over 8 weeks, as well as increases in positive affect
and declines in perceived stress, negative affect, state and trait anxiety, and
rumination relative to controls. Further, enhanced mindfulness was associ-
ated with declines in anxiety and distress and a reduced tendency to use
rumination to regulate emotion. Research has also begun to test such asso-
ciations in clinical populations. In an uncontrolled MBSR study with cancer
patients,
Brown and Ryan (2003)
found that increases in MAAS-assessed trait mindfulness were related to declines in stress as well as anxiety, depressive
symptoms, and other indicators of mood disturbance (cf.
Carlson & Brown,
In sum, preliminary trait-based research suggests that mindfulness is asso-
ciated with a variety of affective (and cognitive) indicators of well-being,
while both trait- and state-based research suggests that mindfulness is asso-
ciated with more pleasant affect and, in particular, less unpleasant affective
experience. Those higher in dispositional and state mindfulness appear to
experience unpleasant affect less intensely on a day-to-day basis, and when
in a mindful state, individuals react less intensely to emotionally provocative
stimulation. This lower reactivity, combined with initial evidence for quicker
74
Kirk Warren Brown and Shari Cordon
recovery from induced unpleasant (sad) moods, suggests that mindfulness
promotes more efficient emotion regulation, which may help to explain
the more positive emotional states associated with mindfulness. In turn,
this research also offers support for a variety of theories emphasizing the
importance of attentional sensitivity to psychological and other cues for self-
regulated functioning (e.g.,
Baumeister, Heatherton & Tice, 1994;
Carver & Scheier,
1998;
Deci & Ryan, 1985).
Mindfulness and Affective Processes
Research on the affective processes underlying the apparent salutary emo-
tional correlates of mindfulness is even more recent than that focused on
core affect, specific emotional content, and cognitive appraisals, but the few
available studies are worth noting, particularly because they help to cor-
roborate the research on mindfulness and subjective emotional experiences
described already as well as suggest neural substrates for them.
Emotional processes involve an array of diverse, correlated neurologi-
cal processes
(Anderson, 2007),
but two areas of the brain – the amyg-
dala and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) – appear to be important to both the
experience and the regulation of emotion. There is indication that amyg-