Read Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness Online
Authors: Fabrizio Didonna,Jon Kabat-Zinn
Tags: #Science, #Physics, #Crystallography, #Chemistry, #Inorganic
dala activation is associated with negative emotional experience (particu-
larly fear), perhaps by increasing perceptual sensitivity to negative stimuli
(Barrett, Bliss-Moreau, Duncan, Rauch, & Wright, 2007).
Activation in the
PFC, particularly in lateral and dorsal regions, has been associated with
both decreased amygdala activation and the deliberate diminishment of neg-
ative emotional responses (i.e., emotion regulation), perhaps via ventral
and medial PFC regions
(Phelps, 2006;
Urry et al., 2006).
Together, analysis of amygdala and PFC regions provides a window into the processes
of emotional reactivity and regulation, thereby opening opportunities to
study the neural correlates of mindful processing of emotionally provocative
stimuli.
In a study addressing this topic, Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, and Lieber-
man
(2007)
examined whether more mindful individuals would show less
reactivity to emotionally threatening (negative) picture stimuli, as measured
by fMRI-assessed amygdala activation and stronger regulation of emotional
responses through prefrontal cortical mechanisms. The study found that, rel-
ative to those lower in mindfulness, higher MAAS scorers were less reactive
to threatening emotional stimuli, as indicated by an attenuated bilateral amyg-
dala response and greater prefrontal cortical activation (in dorsomedial, left
and right ventrolateral, medial, and right dorsolateral PFC) while labeling
those stimuli. Also, a stronger inverse association between these areas of the
PFC and the right amygdala was found among higher MAAS scorers. This lat-
ter result suggesting a greater emotion regulatory capacity through mindful-
ness may come through enhanced prefrontal cortical inhibition of amygdala
responses.
Ochsner, Bunge, and Gross (2002)
have suggested that this pattern of activations may be associated with a “turning down” of evaluation
processes, thus switching from an emotional mode of stimulus analysis to an
unemotional one. This is consistent with the receptive, non-evaluative phe-
nomenology of mindfulness described already, in which objects and events
Chapter 4 Phenomenology and Emotional Correlates of Mindfulness
75
in focal attention are simply observed, without attempts to alter or ana-
lyze them.
There is also initial evidence that mindfulness can diminish reactivity to
threat and subsequent distress in social situations, and in particular that
which commonly arises when connectedness is lost due to social exclusion,
an experience that people are highly motivated to avoid (e.g.,
Allen & Knight,
2005).
Creswell, Eisenberger, and Lieberman (2007)
tested whether mindful attention incurred protective benefit against distress when facing exclusion
by members of a peer group and whether this greater equanimity in the face
of exclusion was due to reduced reactivity to this form of social threat, mea-
sured by brain imaging of regions known to be implicated in the experience
of social pain and distress.
Undergraduates participated in a virtual ball-tossing game with two other
“participants” (actually a computer) while undergoing fMRI. In the first
task block, each participant was included in the ball-tossing game, while
in the second block, the participant was excluded during the majority of
the throws. After the task, participants reported their perceptions of social
rejection during exclusion. Results showed that MAAS-assessed mindfulness
predicted lower perceived rejection. Further, this association was partially
mediated by reduced activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC),
a region activated during social distress
(Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams,
2003).
These findings are consistent with the study of romantic couple conflict described already, in suggesting that mindfulness predicts a more sub-
dued response to social threat, in this case, apparent rejection by peers, and
that this attenuated response is due, in part, to reduced evaluative reactivity
to that threat.
The findings also provide a window into the role of mindfulness in alter-
ing the expression of self in social contexts. Theory and research suggest
that personal identity, or the self-concept, is strongly influenced by the opin-
ions and reactions of others, and negative evaluative reactions to rejection
occur because the individual’s sense of self-worth is invested in, or contin-
gent upon, validation by others (e.g.,
Leary
,
2004).
However, if a sense of self that is grounded in experiential processing is operational, events like rejection that impinge upon the self-concept may be less threatening than they
otherwise might be (Brown, Ryan, Creswell, & Niemiec, 2008).
While this notion requires further study, there is initial evidence that the
experiential focus (EF) described by both mindfulness scholars and phe-
nomenologists has neural referents that are distinct from the narrative, con-
ceptual focus that is commonly the default mode of processing.
Farb et al.
(2007)
conducted an induction-based study with both MBSR graduates and
novices trained to use two types of attentional focus upon positive and
negative personality trait stimuli designed to arouse self-reference. The EF
entailed a present-centered, non-conceptual attention to thoughts, feelings,
and bodily states, using meta-awareness to return attention to present expe-
rience when distracted by thoughts or memories (i.e., mindfulness). A nar-
rative focus (NF) was characterized by analysis of the meaning of the trait
words and their application to self in an ongoing stream of thoughts that
is also characteristic of rumination, mind wandering, and resting attention.
Among other findings, the study found that EF yielded reductions in midline
cortical region activity associated with NF in both novices and mindfulness
76
Kirk Warren Brown and Shari Cordon
trainees, particularly the medial PFC (mPFC). These mPFC reductions were
more marked and pervasive among those previously trained in mindfulness.
While still nascent, this neural process-based research converges with sub-
jective report-based research reviewed above in suggesting that the phe-
nomenological or mindful mode of processing is associated with diminished
emotional reactivity to negative stimuli and enhanced regulation of emo-
tional response, perhaps through an engagement of attention upon immedi-
ate experience that permits a disentanglement from the conceptual networks
that link subjective experiences across time to promote anxiety, regret, sad-
ness, and other unpleasant, self-referential emotions. This process research
also offers a glimpse into the neural mechanisms that may underlie the more
equanimous subjective experience that mindfulness is theorized to foster
(e.g.,
Analayo, 2003;
Brown & Ryan, 2003).
Conclusions and Future Directions
This chapter had two primary aims: First, we sought to clarify the nature
of mindfulness by taking a “view from within” the conscious mind’s dual
modes of processing experience. Husserlian phenomenology and Buddhist
theories were shown to converge on several points in the description of a
phenomenology of experience within which the subjective nature of mind-
fulness may be better understood. Second, we sought to describe the impli-
cations of this experiential approach to life for a key feature of subjective
experience, namely, emotions. Findings from recent research using trait mea-
sures and state inductions of mindfulness are convergent in showing that this
quality of presence is associated with more balanced emotional content, par-
ticularly a relative paucity of unpleasant emotional experience. Mindfulness
also appears to promote less reactivity to events that can provoke emotional
distress and more efficient regulation of that distress when it occurs. Studies
using neural imaging have begun to offer clues about the cortical and sub-
cortical substrates for the more sanguine subjective experiences that more
mindful individuals report.
These studies of emotional content and process provide insight into the
mechanisms by which mindfulness foster emotional well-being, and by impli-
cation, mental health. Mindfulness is believed to promote emotional well-
being through multiple means (e.g., Baer, 2003; Brown et al., 2007; Shapiro
et al., 2007), and the present review suggests support for the claim that
mindfulness is associated with diminished evaluation of stimuli (or what has
also been called acceptance and non-judgment), perhaps through an imme-
diacy of contact with them. Another explanation for the emotional bene-
fits of mindfulness is the receptive, non-defensive processing of experiences
that present emotional challenges. Studies reviewed here suggest that willing
exposure, a greater willingness to tolerate or remain experientially present
to unpleasant stimuli without cognitive reactivity, may help to explain the
role of mindfulness in producing greater emotional balance and more effec-
tive emotion regulation in the face of emotionally challenging events and
experiences.
These descriptive and explanatory conclusions must be considered provi-
sional, however, because studies of mindful traits and states are still relatively
Chapter 4 Phenomenology and Emotional Correlates of Mindfulness
77
few, existing samples are comparatively small, and most studies have used
correlational designs – all factors that limit the ability to make causal con-
clusions about mindfulness and emotional experience and well-being. As the
field of mindfulness research matures, opportunities for building a firmer
foundation of knowledge are numerous. Two are briefly noted here. First,
better assessment of mindful traits is needed to more accurately reflect the
scholarly descriptions of this mode of processing. In this endeavor, schol-
arship on both conscious states and cognitive science will be invaluable
for detailing the subjective quality of experience that mindfulness involves.
Second, the advent of experimental research to study mindful states offers
excellent opportunities to observe the nature and outcomes of mindful,
or experiential processing in real time. The value of such research lies not
only in disclosing the nature and functional significance of mindfulness, but
also in helping to address fundamental questions about how the conscious
mind processes experience and how such processing may be optimized to
enhance emotional experience and human welfare in general.
Acknowledgements:
Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by
NIH grant R01 AG025474-02 to the first author. We thank Melissa Glennie,
Laura Kiken, Jonathan Shear, and Evan Thompson for helpful comments and
suggestions on a previous draft.
References
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation
following a focused breathing induction.
Behavior Research and Therapy, 44
,
1849–1858.
Allen, N. B., & Knight, W. (2005). Mindfulness, compassion for self, and compassion
for others. In P. Gilbert (Ed.),
Compassion: Conceptualizations, research, and use
in psychotherapy
(pp. 239–262). New York: Routledge.
Analayo, B. (2003). Satipatthana: The direct path to realization. Birmingham, UK:
Windhorse.
Anderson, A. K. (2007). Feeling emotional: The amygdala links emotional perception
and experience.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2
, 71–72.
Baer, R.A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and
empirical review.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10
, 125–143.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using
self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness.
Assessment, 13
,
27–45.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being.
Ameri-
can Psychologist, 54
, 462–479.
Barnes, S., Brown, K. W., Krusemark, E., Campbell, W. K., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). The
role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfaction and responses to relation-
ship stress.
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33
, 482–500.
Barrett, L. F. (1997). The relationship among momentary emotional experiences, per-
sonality descriptions, and retrospective ratings of emotion.
Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 23
, 1100–1110.
Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S. L., Rauch, S. L., & Wright, C. I. (2007).
The amygdala and the experience of affect.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-
science, 2
, 73–83.
78
Kirk Warren Brown and Shari Cordon
Barrett, L. F., & Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion representation and regulation: A process
model of emotional intelligence. In T. Mayne & G. Bonnano (Eds.),
Emotion: Cur-